Nikkie Claims Hugh Blair!
Nikkie is having trouble with her Onid, so she asked me post her six-word final. Nikkie wrote:
Here's my six words on Blair: Invention Smention...Praise Style and Retention
Nikkie is having trouble with her Onid, so she asked me post her six-word final. Nikkie wrote:
Hmmm, Our friend Ramus...
QUESTIONS:
You disappoint me, Enlightenment period. How can a time of intellectual resurgence be so boring? Bizzell and Herzberg say the Enlightenment was “marked by revolutions in science, philosophy, and politics” (791). Okay, that’s fine. I’m no wannabe scientist, and certainly not a real one, but with philosophy—with its tendency toward the abstract—thrown in the mix, I thought there might be something for me here. This is only barely the case.
"Only Six Words?? Rhetoric will starve!!"
this is my kind of final! Astell:
I may have to edit this later, but here's my Gorgias statement for now: "Everything is better with fancy words."
Okay, here's mine.
( cited recently in The New Yorker)
Labels: Rhetoric in six words
Bizzell and Herzberg point out that “language and culture are inseparable, and though it is common practice to forget the cultural forces at work in descriptions of Standard English—that is, white English—it is impossible to forget, when examining the development of Black English, the often agonized relationship between white people and black people in the United States” (1544).
In lecture tomorrow, I’m primarily going to cover content from A Grammar of Motives, but the last question I am going to pose involves an idea from a later work of his, Language of Symbolic Action.
Why does Burke believe that the ratios “scene-act” and “scene-agent” are the most important? What significance do they hold over the other elements in the pentad? (What is the general importance of the other elements?)
Burke’s concept of language was that it did not simply describe truths, experiences, or ideas. It directs us toward seeing some things, and ignoring others. How does this compare with classical rhetoric? Was “framing” such a blatant concept?
Burke spends a deal of time discussing the distinction between action and motion. In nearly all cases, human motivation drives action. Could we have suggested that Burke take a page from Freud? Is Burke leaving enough room with his concept of motion to accommodate the possibility that “A pickle’s sometimes just a pickle?”
What is the importance of Burke’s terministic screens? Is Burke’s metaphor involving various photos of the same object helpful to understanding the screens? How often do we find ourselves adapting our perceptions retroactively?
1. What is the effect of reading a piece that slides from one language to another? Does this draw you in or pull you out?
Today I got to meet Pat Bizzell, which was really exciting. She's quite a fascinating and complex lady: all at once serious yet witty, brilliant and yet down-to-earth. Like me, she's a big fan of Quintilian ("Doesn't he seem like someone you would have loved to have met?" she said), and apparently, she and David Bartholomae are brother and sister. Ha! She said that, and completely fooled me! "Naw," she said. "We've just known each other so long that it feels that way." Apparently, DB made fun of Pat Bizzell and Bruce Herzberg when they published the first edition of _The Rhetorical Tradition_. "No one will want to read that!" He said. Who knew? DB was wrong.
First off, sorry for missing class this morning: a combination of a long day yesterday, a pseudo-emergency call at 1:15am this morning, and an alarm clock malfunction worked against me. That said, it’s good to still have a way to talk to you all. So:
Mr. Francis Bacon is who I am interested in talking a bit about. His theories in psychology that divide the mind into two separate spheres have a delightfully interesting influence on rhetoric. The two spheres, which emphasize productive and receptive operations individually, give a new and important facet of rhetoric, with special attention to the types of appeals, how they work, and new ways to use them to persuade an audience.
What I am interested in looking at is how these two spheres work, and what their idea does to change rhetoric for both the student and the teacher. I don’t want to get too in depth in theory, because sometimes that can be stifling to unadulterated conversation (which is the point of all of this—this blog and some of the ideas behind rhetoric itself, I mean—isn’t it?). Basically, we’ve got a part of the mind that is a receptor—a receiver that brings in data, however presented. Whatever being received is perceived through a lens (preference, desire, likes and dislikes, etc) and then processed by the mind. The other sphere is the producer, which is also affected by the same lens. What we get, then, is two spheres of the mind that are unique to the person and that influence the success of appeals to that particular individual. The two spheres work together, and in so supposedly represent all of the workings that occur in response to exterior stimulus.
When it comes to rhetoric, then, we can use this psychology to explore how appeals work, which is why it’s important at all. Use of appeals to persuade can then be, as a process, further explored by a rhetor so that s/he will be able to use his audience to his own advantage. This is a mix of old and new, classic and enlightenment. Think of it as if Aristotle got himself one of those psychologist’s couches for his oratory.
Okay, so, what does that even mean? Well, my first question is a typical one: does this still apply today? Do we think along these lines, using two spheres of interior thought to influence our use of appeals when creating an argument? Since both spheres can be used by the writer/orator/rhetor to improve his argument, is this kind of thinking still plausible, and does it have a place in the classroom?
Not working for you? Then how about this: The enlightenment saw a lot of smooshing together of psychology and rhetoric. Do you think this is the beginning of that combination, or is it just the first time people are really talking about it? Does the combination make sense? When you write an academic argument, do you think about the psychology of your audience when you write? Should you if you don’t?
What about our students? When teaching rhetoric, do we appeal to their own psychology to teach, and then show them how to appeal to psychology in their own writing?
Finally, since this is really interesting to me, do you see any danger (giggle) in a rhetoric that also plays with the psychology of the reader?
Does anyone feel, after reading the introduction to Enlightenment rhetoric, like you are either on a roller coaster or watching a ping pong match? I mean, yes – there is a lot of movement in one direction: nationalism is driving language from the universal Latin to the particular [national] vernacular, more and more emphasis on the new science of psychology as a way to understand all other fields (including science as much as rhetoric), and of course the growing supremacy of empiricism and science (making, for example, literary examples the best source of “good taste”).
Bizzell and Herzberg explain that Classical rhetoric does reemerge during the Enlightenment, evident primarily (and initially) in the "Ciceronian conception of rhetoric" (792). That is, the five canons were typically the foundation for the study of literature and criticism, writing and speaking, and a "respectable scientific theory of psychological persuasion" (792). I'll focus on the earlier years within this period, in spite of the significance of Smith, Campbell, Kames, Blair, and Sheridan in the eighteenth century--only because earlier work was more provocative, if less applicable to the modern study of rhetoric.
(Even though I'm not in Group 2, I'm writing this in hope that it might spur other thoughts)